and ............ go!
In sentences, words like and
and but link ideas and clauses together. However, in
conversation, they may be used in other ways. For example, compare the two uses of but in the speech of A below:
1) B:
what did you do today
A: we went to the zoo but we didn’t get to see the hippos
B: Oh, that’s a shame
2) B: have you spoken to each other since?
A:
not really. I know part of it is me being sensitive but
B:
that’s why I asked because I was worried about you
In the first example, the but is being used to join the two parts we went to the zoo and we didn’t get to see hippos. It occurs in the middle of the
utterance, with no break in production, and so does not signal to B that they
can start talking. In (2), the but in A’s utterance is produced after a
completed thought, ‘I know it’s me being sensitive’. However, in this case, speaker B begins speaking and takes
their turn in the conversation.
So, what is the difference between these two but’s? How does
speaker B know that they can start talking in example (2), but not in example
(1)?
These are some of the questions
that Gareth Walker wanted
to investigate when looking at what he calls, transitional ‘Trail-off’
conjunctions. These are
conjunctions (such as and and but) which are used after a completed
thought and signal to others in the conversation that the speaker has finished,
thus allowing the transition from one speaker to another.
After analysing 5 face-to-face
interactions between US English speakers, he identified 28 of these
transitional trail-off conjunctions. In analysing them, he found that they had
certain phonetic and visual characteristics in common. For example, the conjunctions tended to
be produced at a lower loudness level than what preceded them, together with a
wide fall in pitch. Walker notes
that these are both characteristics which are common in turn-final speech, indicating
the end of a speaker’s turn.
Therefore, it is not wholly surprising that the conjunctions share these
attributes. In comparison,
conjunctions which are produced as part of the on-going utterance (such as in
(1)) are spoken with less of a pitch drop and are still quite loud in relation
to the surrounding speech.
Walker also analysed the phonetic
features alongside visual data, obtained during camera recordings of the
interactions. He notes that although the phonetic shape of the conjunctions is
the most important factor in determining its function, visual factors are also
relevant. The two most striking
correlations with transitional trail-off conjunctions were the direction of
gaze and hand movement. He notes
that, in 9 out of the 28 examples, the speaker sweeps their gaze across the
other participants, as if surveying for the next speaker in the
conversation. With respect to
gestures, he notes that with transitional trail-off conjunctions, any gestures
made with the hands are completed at the time when the conjunction is uttered
(indicating that the design of the gesture is constructed to fit the total utterance). In contrast, those conjunctions which
do not signal the end of a turn are accompanied by gestures which continue after
the conjunction has been uttered (another indication to the audience that the
speaker isn’t finished yet).
In conclusion, Walker suggests that
more research needs to be done which ties both phonetic and visual approaches
together. In this way, he says, we
can get a better idea of how both these factors work together in the
organisation of interaction (in this case, with respect to the management of
turn-taking in conversation).
__________________________________________________
Walker, G. (2012) Coordination and interpretation of vocal
and visible resources: ‘Trail-off’ conjunctions. Language and Speech 55:141-163.
doi: 10.1177/0023830911428858
This summary was written by Jenny Amos
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.