tut tut? tsk tsk?
We’re probably
all used to hearing alveolar or dental clicks, when the tip of the tongue
briefly meets the ridge just behind the top teeth. Usually this sound is
thought to express irritation or disapproval, or sometimes sympathy. We may
also be familiar with an ‘air kiss’ or what phoneticians term a bilabial click
– when someone makes a clicking sound by bringing their lips together for a
moment. As Melissa Wright points out, though, the idea that
clicks express emotions is based on anecdote and intuition rather than on
analyses of naturally-occurring talk. She performed the first systematic study
of clicks in everyday English talk-in-interaction, finding, for the first time,
a relationship between clicks and the way that we organize talk in spoken
English.
Wright analysed
18 hours of naturally-occurring telephone conversations in Britain and America.
She found 86 bilabial and alveolar/dental clicks in these conversations, used
by 20 different speakers: 13 men and 7 women. The fact that 20 different speakers of different ages and
from different social backgrounds all used clicks suggests that clicks are not simply
idiosyncratic features of one person’s speech.
In fact, Wright
found recurrent patterns in the way that all twenty speakers used clicks. All
the clicks occurred between the end of one conversational sequence and the
beginning of another. There is an example in the box, where lines 1-5 show the
final part of a longer sequence where Norman was telling Lesley about his use
of a dialysis machine. He closes down this sequence with an assessment, in line
6, which Lesley accepts, in line 7. She then offers a final closing token, hm, in line 9, followed by a bilabial
click (the ‘air kiss’ sound, represented here by the ☉ symbol).
After the click she begins a new sequence (lines 9-12) about an
arrangement between Norman and her son, Gordon.
1. Norman: you
leave Wincanton about three o’clock
2. and get back about two in the morning
3. Lesley: hhhh
oh [:
4. Norman: [and work full time on top of that
5. Lesley: oh
[dear
6. Norman: [but it’s a lot
easier no:w hh huh
7. Lesley: yes
I’m sure
(0.2)
8. Lesley: hm: [☉] .hh okay well I’ll tell Gordon
10. and uhm (0.3) I’m sure-and he was
11. going to give you a ring anyway .hh
12. before Sunday
13. Norman: right
you are (.) yes
All 81 clicks functioned
alongside other phonetic factors that typically occur between sequences of
talk. One such factor is a change in pitch: speakers produced the section of
speech before the click with a low pitch typical of closing sequences of talk, and
they then uttered the section of talk after the click with a higher pitch. After
the clicks, the new sequences were often marked with words such as anyway, right or, as in Lesley’s case, okay
well, all of which can demonstrate that the following talk will be a shift
from what came before. The interlocutors showed that the click-initiated shifts
in sequence were appropriately placed, as they consistently treated them as
unproblematic in the interaction: thus in our example Norman’s right you are in his immediately
subsequent turn, in line 13, indicates his acceptance of the topic shift.
Wright’s
interactional and phonetic analysis, then, demonstrates that displaying emotion
is not the only function that clicks have in talk. They are also a neglected phonetic resource that speakers of English can draw on to
organize their talk, functioning alongside other linguistic features to manage
the sequential unfurling of spoken interaction.
_______________________________________________
Melissa Wright
(2011) On clicks in English talk-in-interaction. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 41/2: 207-229.
doi.
10.1017/S0025100311000144
This summary
was written by Jenny Cheshire
Very informative
ReplyDelete