do children use gestures before words to convey actions?
Young children also use what is known as iconic gestures and these are gestures
that either convey actions, such as flapping arms to represent a bird flying, or to convey attributes
associated with objects, such as holding cupped hands together to depict the roundness of a ball. Researchers Şeyda Özçalişkan,
Dedre Gentner and Susan
Goldin-Meadow were interested in finding out whether these types of
gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs in the same way that pointing
gestures pave the way for children’s early nouns.
They videotaped 40 North American children (22
girls, 18 boys) at home with their parents every 4 months from age 14 to 34
months and collected around 540 minutes of observation in total for each
individual child. The children’s families came from a range of backgrounds in
terms of income and ethnicity but all the children were being raised as
monolingual English. The researchers transcribed all the communicative
words and gestures that the children produced. Given the researchers’ interest
on actions and verbs, they analysed only the children’s use of iconic gestures to depict actions.
They found that iconic gestures do not serve the
same function as, for example, pointing gestures do for nouns. Unlike pointing
gestures, which precede and predict children’s first nouns, the results of this
study showed that children do not produce iconic gestures until around 6 months
later
than they produced their first verbs. Instead, the children relied
almost exclusively on speech to convey their early action meanings. On average,
the children produced their first spoken verb at around the age of 18 months
but they did not produce their first iconic gesture until the age of around 25
months.
The researchers suggest four possible reasons why
children produce so few iconic gestures in their early development. The first
is that the gestures may be physically difficult to produce. However, as they
point out themselves, some iconic gestures only involve moving a finger across
space - to indicate something moving downwards
for example – yet even these simple movements do not occur much before the age
of 26 months. Secondly, perhaps the frequency of iconic gestures in parental
input is low so that children are not often exposed to models of iconic
gestures. Thirdly, the researchers suggest that iconic gestures are likely to
impose a greater cognitive load than pointing gestures because there are
conceptual difficulties in producing the gesture itself; in other words,
children have to work out an action (gesture) which is representative of the
action they want to convey, for example the action of hopping two fingers up
and down to represent a rabbit hopping.
Finally, iconic gestures may be more difficult for children to acquire because actions
(and thus verbs) are associated with concepts rather than objects. Verbs
express relations between things, and relational concepts are generally learned
later than object concepts.
An interesting area for future development that
the researchers identify would be to manipulate the input of iconic gestures to
determine whether early exposure to iconic gestures has an impact on children’s
production of iconic gestures and whether those gestures in turn play a role in
children’s later acquisition of verbs. We await the results of such a study with interest.
________________________________________
Özçalişkan, Ş., Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow,
S. (2013). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 1-20.
doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000720
This summary was written by Sue Fox
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.