There are many ways
we can communicate with each other.
During face-to-face conversation, we use more than words to communicate
meaning and emotion. For example,
we can use different types of eye contact and use our hands to make a variety
of gestures.
These types of
non-linguistic communication, when taken together, are often referred to as
‘body language’. But how does what
we say relate to the gestures we use?
Do we sometimes modify our gestures, depending on the types of feedback
we get from the person we’re talking to?
Judith Holler
and Katie Wilkin designed an experiment which aimed to investigate what they
call ‘co-speech gestures’. These
are gestures (i.e. hand and arm movements) which are directly related to what
is spoken at the time they are used.
For example, when saying something like “he just drove away”, a gesture
such as a hand sweeping away from the body may be used. They wanted to investigate
whether the amount of gestures increased when a speaker received feedback (such
as questions) from the addressee during storytelling. In addition, the
experiment was designed to look at how gestures might be modified following
feedback. For example, if
describing an object, you might say “it was oddly shaped” while making a low,
rough gesture with your hands to approximate what you’re describing. However, if the person you’re talking
to questions “how was it shaped?”, your gesture is likely to be repeated, but
in a higher position and a more accurate way in order to give more detail and
meaning to your description. Indeed, Holler and Wilkin identified four ways in
which a gesture may be modified:
1) Gesture
precision (how precise it is in relation to what is being described)
2) Gesture
size (how much articulation space did the gesture ‘take up’)
3) Gesture
space (for example, where in the addressee’s eye-line was the gesture made?)
4) Gesture
viewpoint (was the gesture made, for example, from the story-teller’s or the
character’s point of view?)
In addition to these,
they wanted to investigate the use of ‘deictic markers’ and how their use
relates to a speaker’s gaze. For
example, the use of ‘this’ in “it was this
big” coupled with a shift in the speaker’s gaze to their own gesture signals to
the addressee that they should also look at the gesture for the information it
contains.
The experiment
involved 28 British English females all telling a story relating to a silent
film they were required to watch.
They then told the story of the film to one of the researchers (though
they didn’t know who she was at the time), while the researcher asked four
pre-planned questions at specific points of the story in addition to any
natural behaviour and feedback.
The results showed
that there were no differences in the amount of gestures before and after
feedback, except when the pre-planned question was one which aimed to get
confirmation from the speaker (i.e. a ‘yes’ answer). In this context, the gesture rate decreased after the
feedback. In addition, 76% of
gesture pairs before and after the four feedback questions were judged to be
‘more communicative’ after feedback with respect to the criteria above,
particularly on the dimensions of precision, size and space. These results, Holler and Wilkin
suggest, show how gestures are designed for the recipient and are modified to
give information which complements what is being said.
The results relating
to eye gaze and deictic markers (such as ‘this’ and ‘that’) showed that there
was an increase in the use of these markers relating directly to the gestures
after feedback, as well as gaze shifting to the gestures. Therefore, the increased accuracy and
prominence of the gestures after feedback was directly linked to the linguistic
descriptions, and these modifications were indicated to the addressee through a
shift of the speaker’s gaze towards the gesture. This shows how linguistic communication and gesture are
closely linked in face-to-face interaction - not all meaning is carried by the
words we say, as the more precise and ‘communicative’ our gestures, the more
meaning they contribute.
__________________________________________________
Holler, J. and
Wilkin, K. (2011) An experimental investigation of how addressee feedback
affects co-speech gestures accompanying speakers’ responses; Journal of Pragmatics 43: 3522-3536
doi
:10.1016/j.pragma.2011.08.002
This summary was written by Jenny Amos
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.