If you've ever heard someone talk to a young child, or
have done it yourself, you've probably noticed that it's quite different from how
adults talk to other adults. Research comparing talk directed to infants with talk
directed to adults has consistently borne this out. In addition to using
features such as repetition and “simple” sentences in infant directed talk (or IDS),
‘prosodic’ features such as rhythm, pitch, intonation and stress also vary
significantly, and to beneficial effect. It appears that young children not
only prefer listening to IDS, but they also use its prosodic features in
language learning. IDS stress patterns, for instance, help focus children’s attention
on target words, as well as identifying grammatical class, and understanding word
boundaries. Features of IDS prosody such as intonation and stress therefore
appear to be important for helping young children to acquire lexical, morphological
and syntactic information.
Deborah Herold, Lynne Nygaard
and Laura Namy
have now found that adults also use prosody to help children understand the
meanings of words. They
investigated the use of prosodic cues in the speech of fourteen, native
English-speaking mothers interacting with their individual children. The
average age of the children was 23 months. The researchers investigated six words
with opposite meanings: happy/sad, hot/cold, big/small, tall/short,
yummy/yucky, and strong/weak. Each
pair was illustrated by images that would be easily recognisable to young
children such as, for big/small, a
big flower and a small flower and accompanying sentences which included the
word being investigated, such as “look at
the big one/look at the small one”. The mothers were asked to read and talk
about the “picture book” with their children, and to make sure that they read each
sentence at least once during the session. The mothers’ speech was then
compared with earlier recordings where the mothers had produced each of the
sentences as if it was directed to an adult. The two sets of recordings thus
yielded talk directed to infants and talk directed to adults, for comparison, and
the participants were unaware of the real aim of the study.
Herold, Nygaard and Namy found that mothers systematically
varied prosodic cues such as loudness and duration in order to differentiate
the meanings of the pairs of words. Adjectives such as happy, tall and strong
were all produced more loudly than their opposites (sad, short and weak) (though
the differences were negligible for the three remaining pairs of words). Happy,
hot, big, short, yucky and weak were all produced with shorter
duration than their opposites. Interestingly, happy was the only member
of a pair to feature both greater loudness and shorter duration, which the
researchers hypothesise may have something to do with the communication of positive
emotion. If so, this is a new and significant finding: previous studies have
shown that prosody can communicate something about the emotion of the speaker,
but this research suggests that features of IDS prosody can provide information about positive and negative
meanings, even when the speaker is not actively experiencing those emotions
themselves.
These findings are the first to show that when they are
directing their talk to children, speakers use consistent and reliable prosodic
cues to mark different word meanings. They add to a growing body of work on the
significance of prosody in first language acquisition.
_______________________________________________________
Herold, Deborah S., Nygaard, Lynne C. and Namy, Laura
L. (2012) Say it like you mean it: mothers’use of prosody to convey word
meaning. Language and Speech 55:
423-436.
doi 10.1177/0023830911422212
This summary was written by Ishtla Singh
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.